STEREOACUITY
AT DISTANCE AND NEAR
Bonita B.P.Wong2, MSc,
Russell L.Woods1, MCOptom, PhD, and
Eli Peli1,2, OD, MSc
1The Schepens Eye Research Institute and Harvard Medical School, Boston,
MA
2New England College of Optometry, Boston, MA
Purpose.
Is there a difference in stereoacuity between distance and near? Previous
studies produced conflicting results. We compared distance and near
stereoacuities using identical presentation formats at the two distances. Methods. Stereoacuity was
determined with the B-VAT Random Dot E (BVRDE) and Contour Circles (BVC)
stereograms presented at 518 cm (distance-habitual) and at 40 cm
(near-habitual) while subjects wore their habitual correction. To equate the
accommodative and convergence demands at distance and near, testing was
repeated at 40 cm with the addition of +2.50 DS lenses and base-in prisms
(near-compensated) that aligned the eyes to the same position as for
distance-habitual viewing. Results. The two stereotests showed
similar findings. On average, stereoacuity was equal for distance-habitual and
near-habitual viewing of the BVRDE and BVC stereotests. Near-compensated
stereoacuity was worse than near-habitual and distance-habitual for both
stereotests. Conclusions. Stereoacuity was the same at
distance and near with normal viewing. The conflict between subject knowledge
of target proximity and the optically-induced relaxation of accommodation and
convergence might have caused poor near-compensated stereoacuity.
§ Stereoacuity
depends on the design of the stereotest. In addition to binocular cues, some
tests contain monocular cues (eg. linear perspective, shadows, parallax, and
texture).
§ The
relationship between stereoacuity and observation distance is not
well-understood. Comparison of distance and near stereoacuities is only
meaningful if the presentation formats are identical under the two conditions
(Table 1).
§ Our
goal is to determine if observation distance affects depth perception in
stereograms.
Table
1 - Review of distance and near stereoacuities from previous studies.
|
Ogle (1958) |
Amigo (1963) |
Brown et al (1965) |
Lit & Finn (1976) |
Zanoni et al (1991) |
Yildirim et al (1998) |
Kaye et al (1999) |
Same stereoacuity
at distance than near |
Ö |
|
Ö |
Ö |
|
|
Ö |
Higher stereoacuity
at distance than near |
|
Ö |
|
|
Ö |
|
|
Higher stereoacuity
at near than distance |
|
|
|
|
Ö |
Ö |
|
Elimination of
empirical factors to depth |
Ö |
|
Ö |
Ö |
|
|
|
Same stereotest
given at different distances |
Ö |
Ö |
Ö |
Ö |
|
|
Ö |
Type of
stereotest(s) |
HA |
HA |
HA |
A |
Distance: BVRDE,BVC Near: Titmus, TNO, Randot Circles
|
BVRDE, BVC Titmus, Randot Circles |
|
HA
= haploscopic apparatus; A = alignment test; BVRDE = B-VAT random dot E; BVC =
B-VAT Contour Circles.
§
A computerized display system, a modified Mentor B-VAT II
SG, allowed four alternative forced choice testing.
§ The targets shown on the standard Mentor monitor (Figure 1) and small monitor (Figure 2) were calibrated to subtend corresponding visual angles.
Distance
Testing at 518 cm |
Near
Testing at 40 cm |
|
|
Figure 1. Standard Mentor Monitor (19.3 x 25.0 cm) (Mentor O & O, Norwell, MA) |
Figure 2. Small
Monitor (1.4 x 2.0 cm) (Model 1M180P45, Thomas Electronics, NJ) |
§
Identical presentation formats were used for distance and
near testing.
§
Range
of disparities in stereotests: 141 to 9 seconds of arc.
§
Four-alternative,
forced-choice testing using a 1 up/1 down procedure.
§
Stereoacuity
= 62.5% correct (Probit analysis). The Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test was used to
determine statistical significance.
Random Dot Stereotest (BVRDE) |
Contour
Circles Stereotest (BVC) |
|
|
Figure 3. A 6/56 (20/188) Tumbling E in a random dot pattern with 71 second dot sizes. The task was to identify the orientation of the letter as up, down, right, or left. |
Figure 4. Four black 6/14 (20/47) circles on a white background. The task was to identify the circle that appears to be in front of the others (up, down, left or right). |
(with habitual Rx)
(with habitual Rx)
Near-compensated at 40 cm
(16 in) (with +2.50 D lenses and BI
prisms)
Figure 5. For the near-prism condition, the power of plus
lenses to equate accommodation
(Diopters = 1/V2) and base-in (BI) prisms to equate convergence
demand (Prism = 100 tan [2(tan-1(PD/2V2) - tan-1(PD/2V1))])
were determined for each subject based on his/her interpupillary distance (PD).
§
Twelve
subjects were selected based on age, refractive status, and oculomotor
function.
Age
|
18
to 35 years old |
Refractive error |
£ -3.00 DS and -1.50 DC |
VA |
Distance: 20/20 or better Near: 20/20 or better |
Habitual Rx |
± 0.50 DS of best correction |
Anisometropia |
< 0.75 DS |
Eye suppression |
No
eye suppression 2 lines above best VA
|
Lateral
phoria*
|
Distance:
£
3.0 D
of ESO or 5.0 D
EXO Near: £ 7.0 D ESO or 13.0 D EXO |
Vertical
phoria*
|
Distance & Near: £ 1.0 D |
Horizontal fixation disparity |
Distance*: £ 2 minarc Near: £ 10 minarc |
Vertical fixation disparity* |
Distance: £ 1 minarc Near: £ 1 minarc |
§ Measured stereoacuity was
better with BVC than BVRDE.
§ Both BVRDE and BVC tests
showed similar trends in distance and near stereoacuities.
|
|
A |
B |
Figure 6. Distribution of stereoacuity for BVRDE (A) and BVC (B) under the three testing conditions of distance-habitual, near-habitual, and near-compensated (n=12). |
Was there a difference between distance and near
stereoacuities? NO.
Distance-habitual
and near-habitual stereoacuities were equal (BVRDE, p=0.43; BVC, p= 0.79)
(Figure 6).
Did relaxation of accommodation and convergence affect
normal stereoacuity? Yes.
Near-compensated
stereoacuity was significantly worse than near-habitual (BVRDE, p=0.005; BVC,
p=0.004) and distance-habitual stereoacuity (BVRDE, p=0.05; BVC, p=0.003)
(Figure 6).
Did prisms induce distortions that reduced near-compensated
stereoacuity? NO.
Near
stereoacuity with yoked prisms was the same as without prism (near-habitual).
Was poor near stereoacuity associated with horizontal
fixation disparity? NO.
There
was no obvious relationship between stereoacuity and horizontal fixation
disparity. However, Subjects RC and SU both had worse near stereoacuity than
distance and showed a Type III fixation disparity curve (Figure 7).
|
|
Figure 7. Horizontal fixation disparity curves for two subjects who demonstrated worse stereoacuity at near than distance for both BVRDE and BVC stereotests. |
§
Distance
and near stereoacuities were equal under normal viewing conditions.
Near-compensated stereoacuity was worse than both near-habitual and
distance-habitual.
§
The
conflict between subject knowledge of target proximity and the
optically-induced relaxation of accommodation and convergence might have caused
poor near-compensated stereoacuity.
§
When
using head-mounted displays (HMDs), there is a potential for a similar conflict
between target proximity and accommodative convergence.
Acknowledgment
Supported in part by NASA grant NLC2-1039 and T. Becker Fund.
REFERENCES
Amigo, G.
Variation of stereoscopic acuity with observation distance. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 53(5): 630-635, 1963.
Brown, J.P.,
K.N. Ogle, and L. Reiher. Stereoscopic acuity and observation distance. Invest. Ophthalmol. 4(5): 894-900, 1965.
Kaye, S.B., A.
Siddiqui, and A. Ward. Monocular and binocular depth discrimination thresholds.
Opt. Vision. Sci. 76(11): 770-782,
1999.
Ogle, K.N.
Note on stereoscopic acuity and viewing distance. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 48(11): 794-798, 1958.
Peli, E. The
visual effects of head-mounted display (HMD) are not distinguishable from those
of desk-top computer display. Vision Res.
38(13): 2053-2066, 1998.
Yildirim, C.,
H.I. Altinsov, and E. Yakut. Distance stereoacuity norms for the Mentor B-VAT
II-SG video acuity tester in young children and young adults. J. Am. Assoc. Pediatric Ophthalmol. &
Strabismus. 2(1): 255-260, 1998.
Zanoni, D. and
A.L. Rosenbaum. A new method for evaluating distance stereo acuity. J. Pediatr. Ophthalmol. & Strabismus.
28(5): 255-260, 1991.